A Lexicological Study on Animal Fixed Expressions
A Lexicological Study on Animal Fixed Expressions

Abstract

This paper presents the research results of a project A Lexicological Study on Animal Fixed Expressions in Mandarin Chinese, Taiwanese, German and English. Four corpora of animal fixed expressions in different languages were compiled during the research. The research goals include (1) examining the primitive semantic features, (2) mapping of the metaphorical tenors and underlying conceits, (3) the lexical change, and (4) social functions of the animal expressions. The findings include (a) the derivation of animal expressions, (b) the attribute of such expressions, (c) the development of some vehicles, (d) modes of thinking of the speakers. The research result can be found in, for example, Hsieh 2004, 2005, 2006a, and 2009. Animal fixed expressions are used in many languages, if not all. They are vocabulary of values that show human emotion, such as swearwords and terms of endearment. They also reveal different people groups’ social norms, philosophy and ideology. They convey rich information of the speakers and are worth researching on.

Table of contents

    1. Introduction

    This paper presents the research results of a project “A Lexicological Study on Animal Fixed Expressions in Mandarin Chinese, Taiwanese, German and English” (NSC 91-2411-H-218-003) financially supported by National Science Council in Taiwan. Four corpora are compiled during the research, they are, a Mandarin Chinese (MCh) animal fixed expressions (AEs), a German AEs, a Taiwanese mythical AEs and an English mythical AEs. The AEs in the corpora include: metaphors, similes, proverbs, sayings, frozen collocations, grammatically ill-formed collocations and routine formulae, all of which are fixed expressions (Alexander 1978, Carter 1987, Moon 1998), not ad-hoc terms or freely generated phrases, and contain at least one animal name that has metaphorical meaning. The Chinese corpus contains 2980 and the German corpus 2630 written and spoken AEs. The Taiwanese and English Corpora have 254 mythical AEs. The data are categorized by the animal names in alphabetical order in EXCEL. Different kinds of data relating to individual AEs were recorded in up to 12 separate fields.

    This project aims to sketch a figure of how the AEs are derived from the vehicles (the animal names), to examine the primitive semantic features of the collected AEs, and then to map the metaphorical tenors (the meaning of the AEs) to the underlying conceit (the relation between the vehicle and the tenor). On the other hand, we observe the lexical change, the linguistic and social functions of the AEs and at the end the language ideologies. [The findings have been published in various journals or presented at conferences, such as Hsieh & Jucker (2003) and Hsieh (2002, 2004, 2005, 2006a, 2006b, 2007a, 2007b, 2009).]

    2. The derivation of animal fixed expressions

    Wierzbicka (1985:167) proposes that animal terms are developed from the animals' appearances, habits, and relations to people. Our data provide further information. Many AEs are arbitrary inventions (15% in MCh and 9% in German) and have nothing to do with the animals themselves. The arbitrary inventions of the AEs can be from fairy tales (hu2jia3hu3wei1 狐假虎威), superstition (Ich habe ein Vögelchen davon singen hören), from transliteration (xiong2xiong2 熊熊) or loan translation (qian1xi1chong2 千禧蟲), etc. They have their roots in traditional, rural society and language contact.

    The meaning of a word contains a word’s meaning, grammatical properties and our general cultural knowledge about the world (Wittgenstein 1978, Fillmore and Atkins 1992). The same animal appearance or behaviour can be perceived and interpreted differently by different peoples of various cultures. Fig. 1 sketches an image of how the animal words “live” in people’s mind.

    Fig. 1. The derivation of animal expressions

    The corpora further indicate that Chinese tend to generate more AEs from animal appearances and apply them to the basic-need domain (see Table 1), e.g. that a snail carries a shell is observed by Chinese people, thus, wu2ke2gua1niu2 無殼蝸牛 (no-shell-snail – people who are not capable of purchasing houses) is produced, to apply to the basic housing need. On the other hand, the Germans tend to generate more AEs from animal behaviours or habits and apply them to an emotional domain, in addition to applying to basic need domain. That a snail carries its shell is also observed by the Germans, but the behaviour that it withdraws into its shell when encountering danger is the underlying conceit of the AEs: sich in sein Schneckenhaus zurückziehen (self-in-one's-snail shell-withdraw) and jemanden zur Schnecke machen (someone-to-snail-make). They are composed to denote “to go into one's shell” and “to come down on someone like a ton of bricks”, respectively. Table 1 counts the percentages of different types of underlying conceits and the share of metaphorical tenors in the MCh and German corpora[ (direct quote from Hsieh 2004).]


    Table 1. The underlying conceits and metaphorical tenors in MCh and German corpora

    Underlying ConceitPercentage ChinesePercentage GermanMetaphorical tenorPercentage ChinesePercentage German
    Appearance27%21%
    basic need domain
    emotion
    amusement
    society
    work, sport, etc.
    25.8%
    5.1%
    5.4%
    14.2%
    49.5%
    10.6%
    8.4%
    8.4%
    9.0%
    63.6%
    Behaviour25%27%
    basic need domain
    emotion
    amusement
    society
    work, sport, etc.
    29.2%
    11.1%
    5.5%
    11.1%
    43.2%
    10.9%
    13.5%
    4.3%
    3.6%
    67.7%
    Habit18%21%
    basic need domain
    emotion
    amusement
    society
    work, sport, etc.
    22.2%
    9.5%
    5.1%
    16.0%
    47.2%
    9.8%
    14.5%
    4.4%
    5.7%
    65.1%
    Human-Animal Relation21%20%
    Arbitrary15%9%
    Unknown8%12%

    3. The primitive semantic features

    Having been influenced by Labov’s (1973) denotation conditions approach, Wierzbicka (1985) studied animal terms in the way of stating explication that contains many semantically complex words. Goddard (1998) then develops Wierzbicka’s proposal and concludes that, for example, the tiger explication “contains many semantically complex words… they function as units” (p.247), and are “composed directly of ‘primitive semantic features,’” (p.255). The linguistic evidence of these features is, e.g., a game of cat and mouse, a cat-nap, catfight, etc. (Goddard 1998:249).

    The primitive semantic features of AEs are abstracted in this research. Here we take MCh and German wolf-AEs as examples. In MCh wolf stands for +malevolent and +cruelty. An arbitrary feature of wolf assigned by the speakers is +lecherous:se 4lang2 色狼 (color-wolf – sexual maniac) and lang2wen3 狼吻 (wolf-kiss – to be raped). According to Jiyun (The Book of Rhymes), the bei (狽) is an animal of wolf genus. Wolf and bei often collaborate by walking or working together. The blending of wolf and bei is highlighted in MCh:lang2bei4wei2jian 1 狼狽為奸 (wolf-bei-do-evil – act in collusion with each other), lang2bei4 狼狽 (wolf-bei – embarrassed; in a difficult position) and so on[ (direct quote from Hsieh 2005)].

    The wolf in German stands for +greed and +malevolent. Even the adjective wölfisch (wolfish – greedy, cruel) was generated. The combination of wolf and sheep gave raise to several AEs: Wer sich zum Schaf macht, den fressen die Wölfe (who acts like a sheep will be eaten by wolves), ein Wolf im Schafspelz (the wolf in sheep’s skin – the wolf in sheep’s clothing), etc. In reality the predator is after the sheep because it is a simple prey. In the Bible and in fairy tales wolf and sheep appear side-by-side; their relationship represents the contrast [+good] vs. [+evil] or [+weak] vs. [+strong][ (direct quote from Hsieh 2005)].

    As wild animals are hard to tame, people deal with them in a respectful manner. Consequently there are no tiger-AEs referring to the human-animal relations. This is completely different from that of domestic animals. Table 2 lists the primitive semantic features of some vehicles. The percentages in the table indicate the more salient features. Those in brackets are out-of-date ones that can be found only in literature.


    Table 2. Primitive semantic features of some wild animal names in Mandarin Chinese and German

    VehiclesMandarin ChineseGerman
    Tigerstrength/power 24.4%, danger 22.1%, wickedness 15.1%, cruelty 9.3%, leader 12.3%, courage/boldness 7.6%, greed 5.5%, big, great, swallowing, jumping, vitality, proud, significant, valuable, energetic, robust, awfully, auspicious, superstitiousstrength/power 66.7%, courage, hunt, protector, rapidity, gasoline, (jealousy)
    Wolfmalevolence 26.9%, cruelty 15.4%, lecherous, thankless, yammers, cunningcruelty 27.8%, destruction 22.2%, malevolence 16.7%, hunger 16.7%, greed 16.7%, evil, strong, intensifier
    Birdgain 10.3%, loving couple 10.1%, messenger, girl, someone, something, unpleasant person, followers, penis, free, nice voice, timid, stupid, small, inexperienced, parroting, crazy, awkwardly, useless, determination, goal, (sun)comic 11.1%, confidential messenger, free, goal, small, light weight, cute, eat little, rapidity, loosely, unsteady, mad, strange, confusing thought, merrily, sexual intercourse, defect, sacrifices
    Fishprofit 17.1%, fecundity 12.2%, person in danger 7.3%, lover 5.6%, well, swim well, goal, work, chance, ability, someone, something, friend, society/group, message, innocent, joke, (acrobatics)someone 13.7%, profit/purpose 11.8%, event 11.8%, cold-blooded, uncertainty, unreliability, (no intelligence)
    Wormdamage 64%, laze 28%, inferiority 24%, small, insignificant, flattering, hungry, enthusiast, poisonous, disease, scatterbrain, (decomposition, other animal)parasite 25%, small size 20%, defenselessness 20%, trouble 18%, defect 15%, danger 15%, bad conscience 9%, shape of a thread, restless, anger, poor, addiction, mad idea, mystery/secret, disturbing, (wriggling forward, grave)

    4. Lexical change

    Dragon-AEs occupy about 9% of the MCh corpus. While lexical meaning changes from concrete to abstract (Traugott 1995: 32), the lexemes contain long 龍 (dragon) develops in a different way: abstract > concrete, high > low. The semantic element long can now serve as a popular phonetic representation stands for the phonological unit [+liquids] + [-front vowels] + [+nasal consonant] due to the language contact, e.g., sha1long2 (salon) andnai4long2 (nylon). This is a new tendency for many Chinese characters when loaning words from other languages by the way of transliteration. Homonyms play a key role here. A transliteration can be so widely used that it becomes an affix underwent grammaticalization.

    Grammaticalization is observed in animal name usage in both languages. They reinforce the meaning of their heads in the compounds or the phrases and serve as intensifiers, e.g., the Affen in Affenschande (monkey shame- absolute scandal) doesn’t mean “monkey” and the Bären in Bärenkälte (bear-cold – big cold) doesn’t refer to “bear”. They lost or mitigated their own semantic function and work as grammatical units.[ (Hsieh 2007b)]

    5. Vocabulary of values

    The corpora show that about 80% of AEs are used to scorn or warn people. AEs are not used for bad purposes but rather due to the ignorance of animal’s nature (Schenda 1998:13). In other words, the metaphorical vehicles that people adopted to produce AEs and people's knowledge of animals are often based on different cognitive levels. For example, zoological research (e.g., Grzimek 1988:20) reports that pigs are smart, but ben 4 zhu 1 笨豬 (dumb pig; idiot) is a popular AE[ (Hsieh 2004)].

    As a matter of fact, AEs are our vocabulary of values; AEs express positive and negative sanctions in the societies. Praise and reprimand help the process of adaptation to the norms and rules of the society. For instance, when one is called a falscher Hund (a false dog – a false man; a liar), he should know that his behaviour is considered to be “false, underhanded, insidious” and should change his attitude accordingly. When being called a gen1pi4chong2 跟屁蟲 (follow-butt-worm – bluebottle) one knows that it is improper to cling to someone like a leech[ (Hsieh 2004)].

    6. Semantic, social functions and language ideology

    Why do we need AEs? AEs possess semantic and sociolinguistic functions. One semantic function is that we need metaphorical vehicles to express our social norms and emotions. The animals live close to men and we are close biologically too. Human beings make good use of the names of other animal species and create AEs to express our values or criticisms in a poetic, entertaining and imaginative way. On the other hand, AEs are the terms to convey emotions. There are many secular benedictions and terms of endearments in the form of AEs. Secular benedictions satisfy peoples’ superstition or help express their imagination. Endearments help convey emotions[ (see more in Hsieh 2006a)].

    AEs also show the different ways of thinking and traditional philosophy of the peoples, e.g., the Confucianism, Taoism, Buddhism in a Chinese speaking society and Christianity in Germany. AEs indicate that the MCh speakers tend to think group-centrically while the Germans think individualistically or egocentrically.

    Literature

    1. Alexander, R. J. 1978. Fixed expressions in English: a linguistic, psycholinguistic, sociolinguistic and didactic study (part 1). Anglistik und Englischunterricht 6: 171-188.
    2. Carter, R. 1987. Vocabulary. Allen and Unwin, London.
    3. Fillmore, Charles, J. and Atkins, Beryl. T. 1992. Toward a Frame-Based Lexicon: The Semantics of RISK and its Neighbours. in A. Lehere and E. F. Kittay (eds.). Frames, Fields and Contrasts. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
    4. Fontecha, A. F., Catalan, R. M. J. 2003. Semantic derogation in animal metaphor: a contrastive-cognitive analysis of two male-female examples in English and Spanish. Journal of Pragmatics 35: 771-797.
    5. Goddard, Cliff. 1998. Semantic Analysis: A Practical Introduction. Oxford, New York: Oxford University Press.
    6. Grzimek B. 1988. Grzimeks Enzyklopädie (Band 5) – Säugetiere. München, Kindler Verlag.
    7. Hsieh, Shelley [Ching-yu (謝菁玉)]. 2001. Teirmetaphern im Modernen Chinesischen und Deutschen: Eine Vergleichende Semantische und Soziolinguistische Studie. Doctorial dissertation. Tübingen University, Tübingen, Germany.
    8. [---. 2002. “Betrachtungspunkte zu Tieren und Interpretationen der Tiermetaphern: Kulturunterschiede zwischen Chinesen und Deutschen” (Observing the Animals and Interpreting the Animal Metaphors: Cultural Differences between Chinese and German). In Selected Papers from the Symposium: Combining Multi-Culture with English Language Teaching in Vocational Universities and Senior High School. Tainan: Southern Taiwan University of Technology, pp. 117-143.]
    9. [---. 2004. “]The Corpora of Mandarin Chinese and German Animal Fixed Expressions: A Cognitive Semantic Application[” University Centre for Computer Corpus Research on Language Technical Papers, Special Issue, England: The University of Birmingham, 2004(18): 27-35.]
    10. [---. 2005. “Life form concepts: Animal expressions in Mandarin Chinese and German” Journal of Southern Taiwan University of Technology 30:185-200.]
    11. [---. 2006a. “A corpus based study on animal expressions in Mandarin Chinese and German” Journal of Pragmatics 38(12):2206-2222.]
    12. [---. 2006b. “German culture in German emotion expressions: A study of animal and plant fixed expressions” Studies in International Cultures 2(1):1-24.]
    13. [---. 2007a. Konzepte der Tiermetaphern im Chinesischen und Deutschen. Deutsch-taiwanische Hefte (Journal für deutsche Studien) 13: 190-211.]
    14. [---. 2007b. A diachronic study of dragon lexemes in Mandarin Chinese: Lexical change and semantic development. Intergrams: Studies in Languages & Literatures 8.1]
    15. [---. 2009. Embodiment in Languages (1): Human, animal and plant expressions. Taipei: Bookman.]
    16. [Hsieh, Shelley Ching-yu and Jucker, Jan. 2003. “The Corpora of Mandarin Chinese and German Animal Expressions: An Application of Cognitive Metaphors and Language Change” In Proceedings of the Corpus Linguistics 2003 main conference, England: Lancaster University, pp. 332-341.]
    17. Labov, William. 1973. The boundaries of words and their meanings. In: Bailey, C. J., Shuy, R. (Eds.), New Ways of Analyzing Variation in English, Georgetown University Press, Washington. pp. 340-373.
    18. Low, Ge, D. 1988. On teaching metaphor. Applied Linguistics 9(2):125-147.
    19. Moon, R. 1998. Fixed Expressions and Idioms in English. Clarendon Press, Oxford.
    20. Newmark, P. 1988. Approaches to Translation. Prentice Hall, Hemel Hempstead.
    21. Schenda R. 1998. Who's Who der Tiere – Märchen, Mythen und Geschichten. München, Deutscher Taschenbuch Verlag.
    22. Traugott E. C. 1995. Subjectification in grammaticalisation. In D. Stein and S. Wright (eds.) Subjectivity and Subjectification CUP, Cambridge.
    23. Wierzbicka, Anna. 1985. Lexicography and Conceptual Analysis. Karoma, Ann Arbor.
    24. Wittgenstein, L. 1978. Philosophical Investigations. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
    Shelley Ching-yu Hsieh. Date: XML TEI markup by WAB (Rune J. Falch, Heinz W. Krüger, Alois Pichler, Deirdre C.P. Smith) 2011-13. Last change 18.12.2013.
    This page is made available under the Creative Commons General Public License "Attribution, Non-Commercial, Share-Alike", version 3.0 (CCPL BY-NC-SA)

    Refbacks

    • There are currently no refbacks.